Two types of mathematician, yet again

I’ve recently been browsing through Season 1 of Venkatesh Rao’s Breaking Smart newsletter. I didn’t sign up for this originally because I assumed it was some kind of business thing I wouldn’t care about, but I should have realised it wouldn’t stray far from the central Ribbonfarm obsessions. In particular, there’s an emphasis on my favourite one: figuring out how to make progress in domains where the questions you are asking are still fuzzy and ambiguous.

‘Is there a there there? You’ll know when you find it’ is explicitly about this, and even better, it links to an interesting article that ties in to one of my central obsessions, the perennial ‘two types of mathematician’ question. It’s just a short Wired article without a lot of detail, but the authors have also written a pop science book it’s based on, The Eureka Factor. From the blurb it looks very poppy, but also extremely close to my interests, so I plan to read it. If I had any sense I’d do this before I started writing about it, but this braindump somehow just appeared anyway.

The book is not focussed on maths – it’s a general interest book about problem solving and creativity in any domain. But it looks like it has a very similar way of splitting problem solvers into two groups, ‘insightfuls’ and ‘analysts’. ‘Analysts’ follow a linear, methodical approach to work through a problem step by step. Importantly, they also have cognitive access to those steps – if they’re asked what they did to solve the problem, they can reconstruct the argument.

‘Insightfuls’ have no such access to the way they solved the problem. Instead, a solution just ‘pops into their heads’.

Of course, nobody is really a pure ‘insightful’ or ‘analyst’. And most significant problems demand a mixed strategy. But it does seem like many people have a tendency towards one or the other.


A nice toy problem for thinking about how this works in maths is the one Seymour Papert discusses in a fascinating epilogue to his Mindstorms book. I’ve written about this before but I’m likely to want to return to it a lot, so it’s probably worth writing out in a more coherent form that the tumblr post.

Papert considers two proofs of the irrationality of the square root of two, “which differ along a dimension one might call ‘gestalt versus atomistic’ or ‘aha-single-flash-insight versus step-by-step reasoning’.” Both start with the usual proof by contradiction: let \sqrt{2} = \frac{p}{q}, a fraction expressed in its lowest terms, and rearrange it to get

p^2 = 2 q^2 .

The standard proof I learnt as a first year maths student does the job. You notice that p must be even, so you write it as p=2r, sub it back in and notice that q is going to have to be even too. But you started with the fraction expressed in its lowest terms, so the factors shouldn’t be there and you have a contradiction. Done.

This is a classic ‘analytical’ step-by-step proof, and it’s short and neat enough that it’s actually reasonably satisfying. But I much prefer Papert’s ‘aha-single-flash-insight’ proof.

Think of p as a product of its prime factors, e.g. 6=2*3. Then p^2 will have an even number of each prime factor, e.g. 36=2*2*3*3.

But then our equation p^2 = 2 q^2 is saying that an even set of prime factors equals another even set multiplied by a 2 on its own, which makes no sense at all.

This proof still has some step-by-step analytical setup. You follow the same proof by contradiction method to start off with, and the idea of viewing p and q as prime factors still has to be preloaded into your head in a more-or-less logical way. But once you’ve done that, the core step is insight-based. You don’t need to think about why the original equation is wrong any more. You can just see it’s wrong by looking at it. In fact, I’m now surprised that it didn’t look wrong before!

For me, all of the fascination of maths is in this kind of insight step. And also most of the frustration… you can’t see into the black box properly, so what exactly is going on?


My real, selfish reason for being obsessed with this question is that my ability to do any form of explicit step-by-step reasoning in my head is rubbish. I would guess it’s probably bad compared to the average person; it’s definitely bad compared to most people who do maths.

This is a major problem in a few very narrow situations, such as trying to play a strategy game. I’m honestly not sure if I could remember how to draw at noughts and crosses, so trying to play anything with a higher level of sophistication is embarrassing.

Strategy games are pretty easy to avoid most of the time. (Though not as easy to avoid as I’d like, because most STEM people seem to love this crap 😦 ). But you’d think that this would be a serious issue in learning maths as well. It does slow me down a lot, sometimes, when trying to pick up a new idea. But it doesn’t seem to stop me making progress in the long run; somehow I’m managing to route round it. So what I’m trying to understand when I think about this question is how I’m doing this.

It’s hard to figure it out, but I think I use several skills. One is simply that I can follow the same chains of reasoning as everyone else, given enough time and a piece of paper. It’s not some sort of generalised ‘inability to think logically’, or then I suppose I really would be in the shit. Subjectively at least, it feels more like the bit of my brain that I have access to is extremely noisy and unfocussed, and has to be goaded through the steps in a very slow, explicit way.

Another skill I enjoy is building fluency, getting subtasks like bits of algebraic manipulation ‘under my fingers’ so I don’t have to think about them at all. This is the same as practising a musical instrument and I’m familiar with how to do it.

But the fun one is definitely insight. Whatever’s going on in Papert’s ‘aha-single-flash-insight’ is the whole reason why I do maths and physics, and I wish I understood it better. I also wish there were more resources for learning how to work with it, as I’m pretty sure it’s my main trick for working round my poor explicit reasoning skills.


My workflow for trying to understand a new concept is something like:

  1. search John Baez’s website in the hope that he’s written about it;
  2. google something like ‘[X] intuitively’ and pick out any fragments of insight I can find from blog posts, StackExchange answers and lecture notes;
  3. (back when I had easy access to an academic library) pull a load of vaguely relevant books off the shelf and skim them;
  4. resign myself to actually having to think for myself, and work through the simplest example I can find.

The aim is always to find something like Papert’s ‘set of prime factors’ insight, some key idea that makes the point of the concept pop out. For example, suppose I want to know about the Maurer-Cartan form in differential geometry, which has this fairly unilluminating definition on Wikipedia:

maurercartan

Then I’m done at step 1, because John Baez has this to say:

Let’s start with the Maurer-Cartan form. This is a gadget that shows up in the study of Lie groups. It works like this. Suppose you have a Lie group G with Lie algebra Lie(G). Suppose you have a tangent vector at any point of the group G. Then you can translate it to the identity element of G and get a tangent vector at the identity of G. But, this is nothing but an element of Lie(G)!

So, we have a god-given linear map from tangent vectors on G to the Lie algebra Lie(G). This is called a “Lie(G)-valued 1-form” on G, since an ordinary 1-form eats tangent vectors and spits out numbers, while this spits out elements of Lie(G). This particular god-given Lie(G)-valued 1-form on G is called the “Maurer-Cartan form”, and denoted ω.

This requires a lot more knowledge going in than the square root of two example, because I need to know what a Lie group and a Lie algebra and a 1-form are to get any use out of it. But if I’ve already struggled through getting the necessary insights for those things, I now have exactly the further insight I need: if you can translate your tangent vector back to the identity it’ll magically turn into a Lie algebra element, so then you’ve got yourself a map between the two sorts of things. And if I don’t know what a Lie group and a Lie algebra and a 1-form are, it’s pointless me trying to learn about the Maurer-Cartan form anyway.

Unfortunately, nobody has locked John Baez in a room and made him write about every topic in mathematics, so normally I have to go further down my algorithm, and that’s where things get difficult. There’s surprisingly poor support for an insight-based route through maths. If you want insights you have to dig for them, one piece at a time.

Presumably this is at least partially a hangover of the twentieth century’s obsession with formalism. Insights don’t look like proper logical maths with all the steps written out. You just sort of look at them, and the work’s mostly being done by a black box in your head. So this is definitely not a workflow I was taught by anyone during my maths degree; it’s one I improvised over time so that I could get through it anyway, when presented with definitions as opaque as the one from the Wikipedia article.

I’m confident that we can do better. And also that we will, as there seems to be an increasing interest in developing better conceptual explanations. I think Google’s Distill project and their idea of ‘research debt’ is especially promising. But that article’s interesting enough that it should really be a separate post sometime.

6 thoughts on “Two types of mathematician, yet again

  1. robotpliers June 8, 2017 / 5:09 pm

    Some aspects of “insight” that occur to me after reading your post:

    1) Insightful thought as merging “lower connectivity” concepts. The first proof for sqrt(2) fits within the well-established boundaries of algebra: A substitution, plus some reasoning about reducing fractions and even numbers. The second requires one to realize that the properties of prime numbers are a useful tool here. It involves more lateral thinking. Whether this is due to differences in brain structure, formal education, and/or more, I don’t know.

    2) Insightful explanations edit information to enhance clarity, plus address “why.” This is your Baez example. Insight thinking tends to use “why” reasoning to cut through unnecessary elaboration, while analytical thinking tends to use “how” more prominently and is perhaps more vulnerable to carrying around details stripped of context, making it more confusing to outsiders.

    3) Insight related to creativity. Some people are very smart but not all that creative, and so analytical thinking is more natural for them. They are amazing at applying the routines of thought that they’ve been exposed to. Some other people are very creative but may or may not be quite as smart. They may have trouble using analytical methods, but they are able to come up with interesting answers or observations, to bridge ideas that most people cannot. Insight as a separate track of ability, correlated with IQ but not identical to it.

    Combine (2) and (3) and you can get a possible reason that some very smart people are bad at explaining things to others: They can focus on analytical/”how” thinking and absorb all the enormous details absent context and can successfully use them, but they are bad at insightful/”why” thinking and so cannot cut down the detail and provide the important information needed to explain a concept to less smart (or just less experienced) people.

    Insight also seems to rely more heavily on intuition, avoiding conscious step-by-step reasoning and just having thoughts emerge in your head mostly fully formed. At least, that’s how it works for me. I don’t directly see the causation of the ideas, but they just emerge, like boats sailing out fog, and only once I can grapple with them in the light can I start linking them back to other ideas in my head.

    Also, strategy games are great :).

    Like

    • drossbucket June 14, 2017 / 7:53 pm

      Yes, agree with all of this (apart from the strategy games of course! I seem to be an outlier on that one though…)

      The bit about some smart people being bad at explanations is really interesting. It’s true that some people are spectacularly bad at explaining their work, and it does often seem to correlate with being very detail-oriented.

      It’s funny, though, because in some ways you might expect it to be the other way round! After all, in this ‘analytical’/’insightful’ distinction, it’s the more ‘analytical’ people who are supposed to have cognitive access to what they did. Except, I suppose, that what they did was a bunch of steps, of the sort that doesn’t feel very satisfying as an explanation. Whereas the ‘insightful’ people just have something pop into their head, that somehow *is* satisfying.

      A lot more to think about there.

      > I don’t directly see the causation of the ideas, but they just emerge, like boats sailing out fog, and only once I can grapple with them in the light can I start linking them back to other ideas in my head.

      Yeah, that’s a good description! Funny how we all seem to reach for the fog analogy.

      Like

  2. mindlevelup June 29, 2017 / 4:46 pm

    I’m curious to see what you might think about these “story proofs”:
    http://chalkdustmagazine.com/features/proof-by-storytelling/

    For more dichotomies in thinking that sort of mirrors analytic vs insight, have you seen the focused vs diffuse mode distinction by Barbara Oakley?
    https://www.brainscape.com/blog/2016/08/better-learning-focused-vs-diffuse-thinking/

    Also, being able to bridge inferential distances seems very relevant to teaching / learning: http://everydayutilitarian.com/essays/why-its-hard-to-explain-things-inferential-distance/
    (i.e. answering the question of “What is the next thing I need to grasp?”)

    In a way, “insight” often seems to be a higher-level encoding in a format more palatable to general human mental consumption. The language we’re using is closer to what we’re used to for typical conversation, so we can “cheat” by sneaking in the formal arguments using more typical words / language. (And then things like logical implications are more implicit than explicit.)

    Like

    • drossbucket June 30, 2017 / 6:36 pm

      Thanks for the Chalkdust link! I was a maths student at UCL and had heard the students there had now set up some kind of magazine, but hadn’t got round to looking it up. I’m impressed! Those story proofs look good, and likely to be a lot more enlightening than proofs by induction, though I haven’t gone through the details yet. Also this article on problem solving looks decent:

      http://chalkdustmagazine.com/features/problem-solving-101/

      I hadn’t seen the focussed/diffuse distinction until I read your post the other day but I like the idea a lot, as I’m well aware from a practical point of view of how important switching between the two is when you’re stuck. I try to follow a work pattern something like the one in the link below, and the focussed/diffuse idea might work well to make some sense of it – you turn up and try and work every day to get the benefit of the focussed bit, but accept the fact that sometimes the results will be crap as you need some more diffuse time to assimilate what you’re learning:

      http://www.tempobook.com/2011/08/17/daemons-and-the-mindful-learning-curve/

      It’s very tempting to match up ‘analytical’/’focussed’ and ‘insightful’/’diffuse’, though I don’t think they’re exactly the same.

      > In a way, “insight” often seems to be a higher-level encoding in a format more palatable to general human mental consumption.

      Yeah, I think explanations with the quality of being ‘insightful’ generally plug in to some cognitive faculty humans are good at, like pattern matching or spatial perception. The ‘prime factors’ square root of two proof has a strong element of this, where you ‘see’ the unbalancedness of p^2 = 2q^2.

      Liked by 1 person

Leave a reply to robotpliers Cancel reply